Skip to main content
Log in

The Social Communication Assessment for Toddlers with Autism (SCATA): An Instrument to Measure the Frequency, Form and Function of Communication in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The Social Communication Assessment for Toddlers with Autism (SCATA) was designed to measure non-verbal communication, including early and atypical communication, in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Each communicative act is scored according to its form, function, role and complexity. The SCATA was used to measure communicative ability longitudinally in two samples of toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Overall frequency of non-verbal communicative acts did not change between the two assessments. However, the form and complexity, the function and the role the child took in the interaction did change with time. Both frequency and function of communicative acts in toddlerhood were positively associated with later language ability: social acts, comments and initiations showed greater predictive association than requests and responses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Initial follow-up at age 36 months indicated a significant trend to increased language ability in a subgroup of the parent-training group compared to the local service as usual group (Drew et al., 2002). However, as the intervention was low intensity (one visit every 6 weeks) and time limited (continuing for a further 6 months only to 42 months of age) and because all the children had been in school settings for several years at the time of the 7 year follow-up it was considered unlikely that any benefit maintained. An analysis of key outcome variables (IQ, language and symptom severity scores) between the N = 11 parent training and the N = 10 local services only followed-up at age 7 years revealed no group differences. Details of this analysis are available from the first author.

References

  • Aldred, C., Green, J., & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication intervention for children with autism: A pilot randomised controlled treatment study suggesting effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1420–1430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 4th ed, text revision. (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, G., Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., & Drew, A. (2000). A screening instrument for autism at 18 months of age: A six-year follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 694–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., Swettenham, J., Nightingale, N., Morgan, K., Drew, A., & Charman, T. (1996). Psychological markers of autism at 18 months of age in a large population. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 158–163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., & Lahey, M. (1978). Language development and language disorders. Chichester: Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aldred, C., Green, J., & Adams, C. (2004). A new social communication intervention for children with autism: A pilot randomised controlled treatment study suggesting effectiveness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1420–1430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bono, M. A., Daley, L. T., & Sigman, M. D. (2003). Relations among joint attention, amount of intervention and language gain in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 495–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Child talk. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrini, B., & Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing in autism: Developmental profile and factors related to change. European Psychiatry, 18, 6–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 91–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 358, 315–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., & Baird, G. (2002). Practitioner review: Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in 2-and 3-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 289–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1998). An experimental investigation of social-cognitive abilities in infants with autism: Clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal, 19, 260–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention, imitation and play as infancy precursors to language and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15, 481–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G., Drew, A., & Cox, A. (2003). Predicting language outcome in infants with autism and pervasive developmentaldisorder. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 265–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Taylor, E., Drew, A., Cockerill, H., Brown, J. A., & Baird, G. (2005). Outcome at 7 years of children diagnosed with autism at age 2: Predictive validity of assessments conducted at 2 and 3 years of age and pattern of symptom change over time. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 500–513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cox, A., Klein, K., Charman, T., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Wheelwright, S., & Drew, A. (1999). Autism spectrum disorders at 20 and 42 months of age: Stability of clinical and ADI-R diagnosis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 719–732.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drew, A. (1995). The issue of directiveness in therapy. Paper presented to College of Speech and Language Therapists’ Jubilee Conference, London 1995.

  • Drew, A., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Slonims, V., Wheelwright, S., Swettenham, J., Berry, B., & Charman, T. (2002). A pilot randomised control trial of a parent training intervention for pre-school children with autism: Preliminary findings and methodological challenges. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 266–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). MacArthur communicative development inventory: Users guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook, E. H., Dawson, G., Gordon, B. et al. (1999). The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 439–484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1957). Meaning. Philosophical Review, 64, 377–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1969). Utterer’s meaning and intentions. Philosophical Review, 78, 147–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffith, E. M., Pennington, B. F., Wehner, E. A., & Rogers, S. J. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70, 817–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, R. (1986). The abilities of babies. London: University of London Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewy, A. L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention in young autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 555–566.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C. & Richler, J. (2006). Early diagnosis of children with autistic spectrum disorders. In T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Early social-communicative in autism spectrum disorders: Relevance to early diagnosis, identification and intervention (pp. 35–59). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C. et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1999) Prelinguistic pragmatic functions as predictors of later expressive vocabulary. Journal of Early Intervention, 22, 205–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning. Cranstong RI: T.O.T.A.L. Child.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P. (2003). Annotation: The neural basis of social impairments in autism: The role of the dorsal medial-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate system. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 793–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990) A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Hogan, A. & Doehring, P. (1996). Early social communication scales. Universty of Miami.

  • Paparella, T., & Kasari, C. (2004). Joint attention skills and language development in special needs populations: Translating research to practice. Infants and Young Children, 17, 269–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynell, J. K. (1985). Reynell developmental language scales (2nd ed). Windsor: NFER Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rollins, P. R. (1999). Early pragmatic accomplishments and vocabulary development in preschool children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 181–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition of childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8, 139–161.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., & Ruskin, E. (1999). Continuity and change in the social competence of children with autism, Down syndrome, and developmental delays. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 64, 1–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siller, M., & Sigman, M. (2002). The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 77–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Perlmann, R., & Nathan, D. (1987). Why routines are different: Toward a multiple-factors model of the relation between input and language acquisition. In K. Nelson & A. van Kleeck, (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 6). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Stone, W., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year-old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 677–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., & Ousley, O. Y. (2000). Brief report: Screening tool for autism in two-year-olds (STAT): Development and preliminary data. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 607–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., Turner, L. M., & Pozdol, S. L. (2004). Psychometric properties of the STAT for early autism screening. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 691–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., & Yoder, P. J. (2001). Predicting spoken language level in children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 5, 341–361.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., Rees, L., & Wheelwright, S (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and non-social stimuli in autistic, typically-developing and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 747–754.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 85–101) Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.

  • Volterra, V. (1981) Gestures signs and words at two years: When does communication become language?. Sign Language Studies, 33, 351–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M. (1986). Ontogeny of communicative functions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 295–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. (2006). Understanding and measuring social communication in children with autism spectrum disorders. In T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Early social-communicative in autism spectrum disorders: Relevance to early diagnosis, identification and intervention (pp. 3–34). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prutting, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364–377.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (1993). Communication and symbolic behavior scales. Chicago: Applied Symbolix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention training for children with autism using behavior modification procedures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 456–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., & van Engeland, H. (1997). Children with a pervasive developmental disorder, children with a language disorder and normally developing children in situations with high- and low-level involvement of the caregiver. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 327–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1976). Diagnosis, clinical description and prognosis. In L. Wing (Ed.), Early childhood autism (2nd ed) New York: Pergamon Press.

  • Wing, L., & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 11–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organisation. (1993). Mental disorders: A glossary and guide to their classification in accordance with the 10th revision of the international classification of diseases: Research Diagnostic Criteria (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zinober, B. & Martlew, M. (1985) Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 293–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L. & Stone, W. (2006). Early screening for autism in clinical practice settings. In T. Charman & W. Stone (Eds.), Early social-communicative in autism spectrum disorders: Relevance to early diagnosis, identification and intervention (pp. 88–113). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from Cure Autism Now, the Guy’s and St. Thomas Charitable Foundation, and the Medical Research Council, UK. We are very grateful to the families who took part in the study and to our collaborators on the CHAT studies: Simon Baron-Cohen, Helen Cockerill, Antony Cox, Vicky Slonims, Sally Wheelwright and John Swettenham. Mike Coleman from the Department of Human Communication Science at UCL provided us with software, advice and support that enabled us to analyse the videotapes.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tony Charman.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Coding Definitions for the Social Communication Assessment for Toddlers with Autism (SCATA)

Each communicative action from the child to the examiner is scored on 4 axes:

  1. (i)

    Form: What the child is physically doing.

  2. (ii)

    Function: Communicative function of the act.

  3. (iii)

    Role: How the communication was initiated: whether the child’s communicative action is spontaneous or in response to some action/communication by the examiner.

  4. (iv)

    Complexity: Whether the communicative act consists of a single form, or whether the child combines forms either simultaneously or sequentially (based on the number of forms that have been scored for that act).

Forms should be coded down if uncertain. For example, if it is unclear from the tape whether a child’s look meets the examiner’s gaze, do not code it as “eye contact” but as “look without eye contact”. Eye contact must definitely be made for a gaze switch to be made be coded.

Only actions directed at the examiner are coded. Actions directed at parents or the camera operator are not coded, and neither are self-directed actions such as vocalising to self. In order for any communication to be coded, there must be some sense that the child is directing it at the examiner or doing it in response to the examiner’s presence. For example, smiling and vocalising at a toy is not coded as communicative if the child would be acting in the same way if the examiner wasn’t there. This doesn’t mean that every communication needs to be accompanied by eye contact, but by some apparent awareness of the examiner’s presence and some kind of engagement or responsiveness.

FORM

Vocalisations

Only one vocalisation can be coded for any one communication—code the highest form observed. For example, if a string of babble contains one recognisable word then code as “single word” only. Laughing would not usually be coded, unless it was definitely used in a communicative way.

Phrase

String of words (errors in pronunciation or grammar do not matter).

Single word

Single, identifiable real word (as long as it is identifiable, pronunciation errors are not important).

Babble

Language-like strings and combinations of syllables, either different syllables or the same ones repeated. Sounds like language but not identifiable as any real words.

Sound

Single syllable vocalisation, e.g. “ba”; also include non-syllabic noises that are commonly used in speech, e.g. “hmm”, and non-speech like vocalisations if they are used in a communicative way.

Looks

Gaze switch

Triadic gaze switch of object-person-object or person-object-person; must make eye contact rather than just looking to person’s face, and the whole sequence must take place within 3 seconds to be sure it is a gaze switch rather than just repeated attention shifts. Because of this, the whole sequence must be initiated by the child: for example, if the child is looking at an object, makes eye contact because the examiner calls his name then looks back at the object, this is credited as eye contact but not as a gaze switch. Eye contact must definitely be made for a gaze switch to be coded; a look from object to the examiner to object that does not meet gaze can only be scored as “look without eye contact”.

Eye contact

Eye contact with the examiner, visible from the tape or commented by the examiner.

Look to the examiner without eye contact

A look to monitor the examiner’s actions or a look to the examiner’s face that doesn’t meet eye contact. the examiner often comments this as a look “through” rather than at her.

Joint attention gestures

Proto-joint attention gestures: the child’s attempts to draw adult’s attention to an object.

Index finger point

A well-formed index finger point to a distal or proximal object.

Index finger touch-point

A well-formed index finger point that touches the target object, picture or person.

Bring and show

Taking an object to adult and obviously attempting to draw attention to it, e.g. by lifting it. This also includes giving an object to an adult by holding it where the adult can see it and waiting for the adult to take it.

Bring and dump

Taking an object to adult for the purpose of showing or giving it, but not making any attempt to draw attention to it or lift it to adult’s line of vision, just placing it on the floor, on adult’s lap etc.

Other gestures

Enactive

Gestures describing an object, e.g. its use, shape or size, or tracing its path with an index finger point in order to describe where it went. Also include here badly-formed points: attempts to indicate the position of an object using a whole hand gesture rather than an index-finger point etc.

Conventional

Learnt gestures, e.g. nodding, shaking head, thumbs up etc.

Instrumental

Most commonly an open-handed “give-me” gesture to request. Be careful to differentiate this kind of communicative gesture from an unsuccessful attempt to reach for and take an object (which would be coded as “act on object”).

Deviant

Abnormal gesture, character or origin not obvious; idiosyncratic gestures that a child uses to communicate e.g. shaking hands by side of body, stereotypies—only code when communicative.

Other

Recognisably communicative gesture that is not clearly abnormal but not well captured by any of the above categories.

Actions

Only code if communicative.

Acts on object

Acts on object e.g. pushes toy car while making eye contact and smiling to share the game with the examiner, or peeps at the examiner from behind chair to play peep-bo. Taking an object from the examiner to request it would be counted here, but the child giving an object would be coded as “bring and show” or “bring and dump”.

Acts on person

e.g. manipulates the examiner’s hand to request an action, pushes the examiner away to signal rejection, leans head on the examiner’s knee for comfort.

Movement

Physical movement around the room, e.g. running away to reject the examiner, or moving whole body to communicate in a way that cannot be classified as a gesture, e.g. leaning towards the examiner, jumping up and down to request an object in the examiner’s hands.

Stand and wait

Used as a form of request: child stands by adult in attentive/expectant posture.

FUNCTION

The purpose of the child’s action, what the child is attempting to communicate. For the purposes of this study we are only coding a single function for each action. If an action’s function seems ambiguous, then code what seems to be the primary function, or code as “not obvious”. It may help to watch more of the tape and see if the communication is repeated and what the outcomes are.

Request

Asking for an object or action; this includes any way of communicating a want or desire, such as nodding head yes when a game is suggested, or taking a toy from the examiner (whether it is offered first or just snatched).

Comment

Remark about an object or event etc, which has no obvious instrumental function but is more conversational, e.g. “look at that”, “I’ve got a car like that”, “I like the bubbles”. For non-verbal children this includes e.g. saying “oh!” when the bubbles appear or “uh-oh” if a toy is knocked over, or catching the examiner’s eye or pointing, in response to a specific object or action that has just happened or appeared.

Naming

A comment whose only function is to name an object, whether spontaneously or prompted by the examiner.

Question

Asking for information, not for an object or action, e.g. “when are we going to play bubbles?” but NOT “can we play bubbles now?” For non-verbal children this is harder to code but can include obvious questioning looks, e.g. a look with questioning face while the examiner is winding the top, as if to ask the examiner what she is doing.

Social/shared pleasure

A communication which is purely social, e.g. a gaze switch and smile when the music box is operated, an affectionate gesture. This includes all attempts to share an activity with another person, except where this could be classified as a comment: e.g. if, the first time a musical toy is activated, the child says “oh” and points to it, that could be classed as a comment, but any definite attempts to draw an adult’s attention to an ongoing activity or to encourage them to share in watching or playing a game should be classed as social. As a very rough rule, in general the first reaction to something counts as a comment, unless the child is obviously smiling and sharing pleasure with the adult; from then on any sharing counts as social.

Monitoring

A look at the examiner that is only for the purpose of seeing what is happening, with no attempt to engage her. When children first enter the room they often look briefly but repeatedly at the examiner before settling into the situation: the length of the gaze and whether there is any attempt to engage the examiner or to respond to her should determine whether these are counted as monitoring or social. Only single looks (with or without EC) can be coded as monitoring, so if the child smiles or vocalises while looking at the examiner that automatically becomes social rather than monitoring.

Response

An action that happens purely in response to another person, e.g. giving an object to the examiner because she has asked for it, making eye contact because the examiner has called the child’s name but without any social engagement. If the function of the communication can be interpreted as anything other than simply a response, the other function should take precedence.

Rejection

Definite rejection of adult. Do not include unawareness or lack of interest e.g. a child wandering off to play with something else, but definite and targeted rejection, e.g. pushing a toy away when the examiner offers it, running away and hiding when the examiner requests child to sit down and play, refusing to give the top when the examiner tries to take it to operate it. Rejections will usually be in response to the examiner’s attempts to initiate interaction, but can also be initiated by the child, e.g. the child approaches the examiner and pushes her away from the toys.

Not obvious

Function is ambiguous but action is definitely communicative.

ROLE

What triggers the child to act, who initiates the interaction

Initiation

Child’s action is not obviously prompted by anything the examiner has done; this includes the child changing the form of an ongoing interaction. For example if he has been slamming the lid of the music box and making eye contact with the examiner each time, each time after the first is counted as a social response because it is the continuation of an ongoing game that both the examiner and the child are maintaining, but if the child then shuts the lid quietly while looking and smiling this counts as an initiation because he has change the nature of the interaction.

Response

Communication triggered by something the examiner has done, e.g. child replies to a question, gives an object when asked to, makes eye contact when the examiner calls his name, takes a turn in an established game of peep-bo.

Imitation

Immediate imitation of the examiner’s last action forms the major part of the communication (e.g. repeating what the examiner has just said while smiling and making eye contact, copying the examiner blowing bubbles as a way of requesting more).

COMBINATION

Simultaneous combination

When more than one form is coded, they occur simultaneously. If many forms are coded as part of a single communication, it’s possible that some will be simultaneous and some will not: if there is any doubt focus on whether eye contact or other looks are simultaneous with other forms in the communication, or if there is no look then focus on whether the majority of the forms being coded are combined simultaneously.

Sequential combination

When more than one form is coded, they are strung together sequentially, but are all part of the same communicative action. Be careful not to include for example an initiation followed by a response, e.g. child holds out an object to the examiner, the examiner says “shall I help with that?” and the child makes eye contact - this would be coded as an initiation of bring-show and a response of eye contact, both single forms.

Single form

Only one form has been coded for that communication.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Drew, A., Baird, G., Taylor, E. et al. The Social Communication Assessment for Toddlers with Autism (SCATA): An Instrument to Measure the Frequency, Form and Function of Communication in Toddlers with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 648–666 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0224-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0224-9

Keywords

Navigation