Skip to main content
Log in

Great Apes Do Not Learn Novel Tool Use Easily: Conservatism, Functional Fixedness, or Cultural Influence?

  • Published:
International Journal of Primatology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Animal culture has been of interest for decades but the concept remains controversial. Many researchers feel that animal behavior should not be granted the label “culture” because the latter includes more than “simple” behavioral variation. In recent years, the study of animal culture has been concerned mainly with social learning mechanisms, often claimed to be human specific, which led to the uniqueness of human culture. In addition, failure to innovate novel cultural behavior as often as humans is usually explained by psychological mechanisms presented as more parsimonious, such as conservatism or functional fixedness. However, it is unclear how cognitively complex these mechanisms are in the first place. Here, I analyze recent data obtained with wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) and wild-born orangutans (Pongo abelii) using the honey-trap experiment, in which individuals must devise a solution to reach inaccessible honey. I compare behavior of apes that developed a tool-based solution in the task with those that did not, and test whether conservatism and functional fixedness can explain individual variation in ape behavior. I find evidence of conservatism, with apes relying on their existing knowledge, and evidence of functional fixedness, with this knowledge potentially preventing them from innovating. However, the apes showed large intraspecific variability. I discuss the two mechanisms through a representational perspective to tackle their cognitive complexity. Understanding how conservatism and functional fixedness interact with ape cultural knowledge to limit innovation of novel tool use appears necessary to understand the full extent of ape cultures and how they compare to human cultures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, J., Weinrich, M., Hoppitt, W., & Rendell, L. (2013). Network-based diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail feeding in humpback whales. Science, 340, 485–488.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amici, F., Barney, B., Johnson, V. E., Call, J., & Aureli, F. (2012). A modular mind? A test using individual data from seven primate species. PLoS ONE, 7(12), e51918.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2015). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518, 538–541.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9), 1–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beran, M. J., Evans, T. A., Paglieri, F., McIntyre, J. M., Addessi, E., & Hopkins, W. D. (2013). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) can wait, when they choose to: a study with the hybrid delay task. Animal Cognition, 17(2), 197–205.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & Hopper, L. M. (2014). Psychological limits on animal innovation. Animal Behaviour, 92, 325–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20, 1478–1483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 187–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, A. J., Marshall, H. H., Heinsohn, R., & Cowlishaw, G. (2014). Personality predicts the propensity for social learning in a wild primate. PeerJ, 2, e283.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2012). Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 126–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Defeyter, M. A., & German, T. P. (2003). Acquiring an understanding of design: evidence from children's insight problem solving. Cognition, 89, 133–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Derex, M., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Social learners require process information to outperform individual learners. Evolution, 67(3), 688–697.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(270), i–113.

  • Fragaszy, D. M., & Perry, S. (Eds.). (2003). The biology of traditions: Models and evidence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franz, M., & Matthews, L. J. (2010). Social enhancement can create adaptive, arbitrary and maladaptive cultural traditions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 277, 3363–3372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G. (1992). The question of animal culture. Human Nature, 3(2), 157–178.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G. (2009). Culture in animals? In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 222–246). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • German, T. P., & Barrett, H. C. (2005). Functional fixedness in a technologically sparse culture. Psychological Science, 16(1), 1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • German, T. P., & Defeyter, M. A. (2000). Immunity to functional fixedness in young children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 707–712.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T. (2013). Historical hypotheses of chimpanzee tool use behaviour in relation to natural and human-induced changes in an East African rain forest. Revue de Primatologie, 5, document 66. doi: 10.4000/primatologie.1690.

  • Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Reynolds, V., Wrangham, R. W., & Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Community-specific evaluation of tool affordances in wild chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 1, 128.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Strimling, P., Wrangham, R. W., & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Current Biology, 19, 1806–1810.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Potts, K., Krupenye, C., Byrne, M.-R., Mackworth-Young, C., McGrew, W. C., et al. (2012a). The influence of ecology on chimpanzee cultural behaviour: a case study of five Ugandan chimpanzee communities. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 446–457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Singleton, I., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012b). Sumatran orangutans differ in their cultural knowledge but not in their cognitive abilities. Current Biology, 22(23), 2231–2235.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Evolutionary origins of the human cultural mind. The Psychologist, 25(5), 364–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Zuberbühler, K., Clément, F., & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hanus, D., Mendes, N., Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2011). Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the floating peanut task. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e19555.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004). Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 53, 3–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., & Galef, B. G. (Eds.). (1996). Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, K. (2009). Animal "culture"? In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 269–287). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2010). Able-bodied wild chimpanzees imitate a motor procedure used by a disabled individual to overcome handicap. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e11959.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter, C., & Byrne, R. W. (2011). The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14, 745–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobaiter, C., Poisot, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppitt, W., & Gruber, T. (2014). Social network analysis shows direct evidence for social transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. PLoS Biology, 12(9), e1001960.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, G., & Fruth, B. (2003). Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. Current Anthropology, 44(4), 563–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, L. M., Price, S. A., Freeman, H. D., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Kendal, R. L. (2014). Influence of personality, age, sex, and estrous state on chimpanzee problem-solving success. Animal Cognition, 17(4):835–47. doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0715-y.

  • Hoppitt, W., Boogert, N. J., & Laland, K. N. (2010). Detecting social transmission in networks. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 263, 544–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hrubesch, C., Preuschoft, S., & van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Skill mastery inhibits adoption of observed alternative solutions among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 12(2), 209–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krützen, M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Culture and geographic variation in orang-utan behaviour. Current Biology, 21, 1808–1812.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Galef, B. G. (Eds.). (2009). The question of animal culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21(10), 542–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehner, S. R., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2011). Can captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) be coaxed into cumulative build-up of techniques? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 446–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1974). The psychology of animal learning. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manrique, H. M., Völter, C. J., & Call, J. (2013). Repeated innovation in great apes. Animal Behaviour, 85, 195–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marshall-Pescini, S., & Whiten, A. (2008). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and the question of cumulative culture: an experimental approach. Animal Cognition, 11, 449–456.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin-Ordas, G., Call, J., & Colmenares, F. (2008). Tubes, tables and traps: great apes solve two functionally equivalent trap tasks but show no evidence of transfer across tasks. Animal Cognition, 11, 423–430.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J. J. M., Antonides, A., Arnold, A.-M. K., Bionda, T., & Koski, S. E. (2013). A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool-orientation measured with group experiments. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 947–958.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa, T. (1999). Communication and tool use in chimpanzees: Cultural and social contexts. In M. Hauser & M. Konishi (Eds.), The design of animal communication (pp. 645–671). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, T. J. H., Uomini, N. T., Rendell, L. E., Chouinard-Thuly, L., Street, S. E., Lewis, H. M., et al. (2015). Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nature Communications, 6:6029 doi:10.1038/ncomms7029.

  • Perry, S. (2009). Are nonhuman primates likely to exhibit cultural capacities like those of humans? In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumptre, A. J., & Reynolds, V. (1996). Censusing chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 17(1), 85–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (Eds.). (2003). Animal innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, V. (2005). The chimpanzees of the Budongo forest: Ecology, behaviour and conservation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sanz, C., & Morgan, D. B. (2009). Flexible and persistent tool-using strategies in honey-gathering by wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 30, 411–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M., & Boysen, S. (1978). Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Science, 201, 641–644.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(11), 477–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentations in an evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 117–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 2045–2415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. J. (2010). Identifying teaching in wild animals. Learning & Behavior, 38, 297–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatory signaling of chimpanzees? In S. Parker & K. Gibson (Eds.), "Language" and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 274–311). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). The question of chimpanzee culture, plus postscript (Chimpanzee culture, 2009). In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 198–221). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, E. J. C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2013). Conformity in nonhuman primates: fad or fact? Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Kendal, R. L., Tennie, C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2015). Conformity and its look-a-likes. Animal Behaviour, 110, e1–e4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., et al. (2003a). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299(5603), 102–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Reniastoeti, D., Knott, C. D., Morrogh-Bernard, H., Nuzuar, et al. (2009). Orangutan cultures revisited. In S. A. Wich, T. M. Setia, S. S. U. Atmoko, & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Orangutans compared: Geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation (pp. 299–309). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., Fox, E. A., & Fechtman, L. T. (2003b). Individual variation in the rate of use of tree-hole tools among wild orang-utans: implications for hominin evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 44(1), 11–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399(6737), 682–685.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 2417–2428.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K. E., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., et al. (2007). Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Current Biology, 17(12), 1038–1043.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). The evolution of animal 'cultures' and social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 362, 603–620.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, S., Yamakoshi, G., Humle, T., & Matsuzawa, T. (2008). Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant-fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 699–702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I thank Gyorgy Gergely for discussions that led to the writing of this article and Fabrice Clément for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. I thank Cat Hobaiter for comments and proofreading of the manuscript. The research leading to these results has received funding from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) of the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under REA grant agreement n°329197. I thank Joanna Setchell and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thibaud Gruber.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gruber, T. Great Apes Do Not Learn Novel Tool Use Easily: Conservatism, Functional Fixedness, or Cultural Influence?. Int J Primatol 37, 296–316 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9902-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-016-9902-4

Keywords

Navigation