We live in an era of big data, where using large numbers increasingly matter and have become an important area of development (Beam and Kohane 2018). The field of neuroscience is certainly no exception. This is becoming evident, for example, in the world of predictive modelling for persons living with chronic neurological conditions, where new and sometimes unexpected insights are being derived from hypothesis-free observations made in very large populations, with many observations on a wide range of different variables, often with very long follow-up periods (Fereshtehnejad et al. 2015; Liu et al. 2017; Schrag et al. 2019). At the same time, it remains crucially important to appreciate the unique importance of meticulous and carefully elaborated observations made in much smaller groups of research participants. And these groups can be as small as a single patient or even a single healthy volunteer. In fact, detailed and judiciously performed studies in single patients with a unique clinical presentation represents a classic neurological approach (van der Velden et al. 2018). Indeed, observing particular clinical signs and symptoms in a person with a focal lesion in the nervous system can be understood, under special circumstances, as a “natural experiment” capable of unravelling crucial information about the normal functions of the central nervous system. Just think of the archetypal example of Phineas Gage, the railroad labourer who sustained a remarkable focal brain injury after an explosion that drilled a metal rod straight through his left frontal lobe (Macmillan 2000). The dramatic personality changes that he manifested subsequently highlighted the critical importance of the frontal lobe in regulating important neurological functions such as character and impulse control.

Our own area of research (i.e., the basal ganglia, Parkinson’s disease and other movement disorders) offers plenty of examples. Indeed, there are quite a few of such observations (Table 1) and some were fundamentally important regarding this current article and the neuroscientific contributions of Professor John Rothwell at this time. For example, the single-case observations of Gowers and Ormerod of patients with movement disorders and hepatic cirrhosis guided Kinnier Wilson to recognize his acclaimed observations. Wilson studied the brain of three patients not only to describe the first basal ganglia disease but also to start reasoning about the function of those nuclei at the base of the brain (Wilson 1912). Wilson allegedly was the first neurologist to try to replicate the impact of lesions in the basal ganglia (i.e. the lenticular nucleus) in monkeys, only to be somewhat puzzled and discouraged by the seemingly little impact that ensued. This led to his conclusions: “Whatever functions the corpus striatum once possessed there is no experimental evidence in apes to show that it exercises any motor function comparable to that of the motor cortex. There is no evidence to suggest that it is a centre for so-called automatic movements. It is electrically unexcitable, and comparatively large unilateral lesions do not give rise to any unmistakable motor phenomena” (Wilson 1914).

Table 1 Examples of influential single-case (n = 1) studies that have greatly contributed to our understanding of the functions of the basal ganglia and that helped to elucidate the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease and other movement disorders

In 1946, in the incipient clinical research unit at the National Hospital for Nervous Diseases (Queen Square, London), George Dawson studied in detail the first patient ever recognized with cortical myoclonus and discovered cortical somatosensory evoked potentials as a result (Dawson 1947). The brain of that patient was later studied by Greenfield who described cerebellar atrophy, which was decades later realized to be an essential mechanism leading to abnormal cortical excitability in cortical myoclonus (Bhatia et al. 1995).

Paul Bucy, a very astute and active neurosurgeon, wondered about the function of the pyramidal tract. He observed how a patient with hemiballism in whom a pyramidotomy was effectively made (as the only possible therapy to stop the severe life-threatening movements) recovered and was left with minimal motor clumsiness on the affected side (Bucy and Keplinger 1960). The striking global recovery of motor power led Bucy to wonder if the pyramidal tract is needed to sustain normal motor control in humans.

Alf Brodal was a well-known neuroanatomist from Oslo, whose book “Neurological Anatomy in Relation to Clinical Medicine” (published in 1969) was a fundamental reference in the 1970s and 1980s. The book contained several other brilliant accounts; in one of these, he provided an incontestable argument against using the term “extrapyramidal” system and, accordingly, “extrapyramidal” syndrome (chapter 4, pp. 182–183). In 1972, while being a visiting professor at Coimbra University, Brodal himself suffered a left hemisphere stroke with among others right-sided classic hemiparesis. He recovered substantially over the following months but was, somewhat to his own surprise, left with difficulties to carry out fine, skilled manual and finger movements with the right hand, such as tying of a tie, particularly when performed without visual control. Indeed, he lucidly expressed his disability as “his fingers did not know the next move” (Brodal 1973). Clearly, his lenticulo-capsular infarction (no neuroimaging available then) had disturbed the motor circuit and the automatic (habitual) system.

In the late 1990s, Bill Langston’s recognition of a single patient (six other similar patients were later identified as well) who subacutely developed severe and irreversible parkinsonism after receiving repeated intravenous injections of 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP), a synthetic variant of heroin, raised firm awareness to the possible role played by environmental neurotoxins in the origin of Parkinson’s disease (Langston et al. 1983). The role of environmental factors in the aetiopathogenesis of PD is still a major area of research (Dorsey et al. 2018). Furthermore, the observation triggered hectic “in vitro” studies to understand the mechanism of action of the toxin and led to a now classic animal model of nigrostriatal neurodegeneration (Burns et al. 1983).

Observations on single patients can also impact on treatments. For example, the striking benefit of thalamotomy against parkinsonian tremor was derived from incidental observations on an initial patient operated by Irving Cooper in whom the thalamus was accidentally lesioned secondary to anterior choroidal artery occlusion (Cooper 1953). The anti-parkinsonian effect of amantadine was serendipitously discovered when the drug was prescribed to a shaking Parkinson’s patient to treat the flu, which was at the time its original therapeutic indication (Schwab et al. 1969). In the current era, it suffices to mention two examples with personal involvement of some of us. One is a patient with typical orthostatic tremor to whom JAO indicated that had she not responded to drug treatments, surgery was always an option since “all tremors respond to thalamic surgery” (sic). Clearly, not a cautious assertion! Two years later, the lady returned ready for surgery. Nothing had been done before for orthostatic tremor and of course, there was no tremor-related activity to record from the Vim as the patient laid down in the operating room. Nevertheless, we decided to place the electrodes in the Vim (recognized by the typical proprioceptive responses) and the patient exhibited a remarkable benefit of deep brain stimulation (Guridi et al. 2008). Several other patients have benefited afterwards. Similarly, it is now appreciated that cycling can be remarkably preserved in patients with Parkinson’s disease, even among those who are completely grounded because of freezing of gait. This notion came from an initial observation in a single patient (Snijders and Bloem 2010), which was followed by observations in larger series of patients (Snijders et al. 2012; Alberts et al. 2011), and eventually culminated in the application of cycling on a stationary bicycle as an elegant way of delivering an aerobic intervention into the patients’ own homes (van der Kolk et al. 2019).

Accordingly, seemingly anecdotal observations such as these, when correctly understood and ascertained, do have clinical and neuroscientific value and emphasise the importance of single-case observations, as a catalyst for new insights and a source of inspiration for future research. As Jan Vandenbroucke elegantly pointed out in a thought-provoking commentary, single-case observations are all but a lack of evidence, but instead represent a particular type of evidence that can motivate and inform further research efforts (Vandenbroucke 1999).

Here, we put forward and discuss the importance of carefully elaborated single-case observations to understand the human nervous system and to throw light onto disease mechanisms. This has been particularly relevant for motor control and the human nervous system. We dedicate this article to Professor John Rothwell, who deserves particular credit for recognising the tremendous potential of such approach over decades of research on the pathophysiology of motor control and movement disorders (Table 2).

Table 2 Examples of single-case studies by Professor John Rothwell and their implications

Remarkably, these case descriptions have had very versatile implications. Given John’s background as Professor of Neurophysiology, it is obvious that much of the case descriptions impacted directly on our insights into the physiology of the human motor system, such as the role of afferent sensory information in shaping motor performance (Rothwell et al. 1982), the contribution of the supplementary motor area (SMA) to motor programming (Dick et al. 1986), the involvement of the globus pallidus in normal motor control and the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease (Kuoppamaki et al. 2005) or the physiology of corticospinal connections (Di Lazzaro et al. 1996). Invariably, the publications contained very detailed clinical descriptions of the patients involved and some of these have actually helped to define the clinical phenotype, for example that of writer’s cramp (Rothwell et al. 1979). Other publications had clear diagnostic implications, such as the detailed polymyography studies of a patient with a suspected functional tremor that eventually turned out to be caused by a structural underlying lesion (McAuley et al. 1998); the methodology used still helps to distinguish functional from organic tremor today. Still other studies helped to clarify the pathophysiology underlying the various types of movement disorders, some of which had until that time been poorly recognised and certainly insufficiently studied. Examples include detailed work on dystonia (Rothwell et al. 1979; Berardelli et al. 1986), various forms of myoclonus (Cowan et al. 1986; Brown et al. 1991; Di Lazzaro et al. 1996; Marsden et al. 2000), chorea (Di Lazzaro et al. 2006), orthostatic tremor (Thompson et al. 1986), hemimasticatory spasm (Mir et al. 2006) and the Tullio phenomenon (Colebatch et al. 1994). Interestingly, even though John is not an M.D. and therefore, not involved in immediate patient care himself, other case studies provided relevant new insights into treatment options for patients with movement disorders. Examples here include detailed studies into the mechanism of action underlying treatments such as lorazepam (Di Lazzaro et al. 2000), botulinum toxin (Mir et al. 2006) or brain surgery for Parkinson’s disease (Obeso et al. 2009). A particularly attractive study—performed in a dystonic patient while playing the piano with and without auditory feedback—provided provocative new insights into the role of music as novel treatment for persons with dystonia (and perhaps other movement disorders) (Kojovic et al. 2012), an approach that had since gained a lot of momentum among both patients, scientists and clinicians (Bloem et al. 2018).

Taken together, John’s impressive body of work should provide a powerful incentive for the next generation of neuroscientists to keep appreciating the value of detailed analyses of single patients, to study the functioning of the human nervous system in health and disease. John’s publications also highlight how such single-case studies can have an impact on patient care as an incidental but desirable catch, by elucidating the mechanisms underlying not only the efficacy but also the adverse effects of invasive treatments. In fact, one could argue that the advent of personalised precision medicine—with its intrinsic call for tailor-made treatments adjusted to the specific needs and profiles of each individual patient (Dzau et al. 2015)—should only lead to a further rebirth of carefully executed single-case studies. This notion is indeed increasingly recognised when it comes to development of personalised treatments, with the advent of new methodologies such as multiple n = 1 trial designs (Schork 2015; Stunnenberg et al. 2018). Single-case studies are therefore not just of historical importance, as we have addressed here, but also continue have great value for the neuroscience of the future.

We like to finish by reminding readers of John’s seminal work on “chronostasis”: the irresistible notion experienced by many when a saccadic eye movement is made to a ticking clock, making the observer think that the second hand takes longer than normal to move to its next position (Yarrow et al. 2001). We have no doubt that this study too was originally triggered by a single-case observation, most likely John sharply observing his own clock—yet another powerful illustration how such anecdotal findings and everyday curiosity can help to advance the field of neuroscience.