Skip to main content
Log in

Screen reading and the creation of new cognitive ecologies

  • Original Article
  • Published:
AI & SOCIETY Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been widely argued that digital technologies are transforming the nature of reading, and with it, our brains and a wide range of our cognitive capabilities. In this article, we begin by discussing the new analytical category of deep-reading and whether it is really on the decline. We analyse deep reading and its grounding in brain reorganization, based upon Michael Anderson’s Massive Redeployment hypothesis and Dehaene’s Neuronal Recycling which both help us to theorize how the capacities of brains are transformed by acquisition of new skills. We examine some of the difficulties in comparing reading using technologies such as the web-browser, the tablet and E-Reader, with reading using the pre-existing print culture. While learning to read undoubtedly changes the brain, we examine what evidence there is for this being tightly tied to particular material substrates and find this lacking. Instead we attempt to situate cognitive changes around the new reading within the context of the specific new cognitive ecologies incorporating both screen and page. This involves a reconsideration of the role of material culture in the cognitive abilities.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In fact, neuronal recycling is supposed to explain not just our ability to read but a host of other skills where our brains appear to have exquisitely developed mechanisms to deal with a particularly culturally dependent ability. See Dehaene (2009, Chap. 8).

  2. Indeed, see Dillon (1992) for an early discussion of the problems of ecological validity in the context of screen reading.

  3. There is a complex but interesting discussion to be had about the nature of technological mediation and transparency. Mangen claims that when reading for narrative, “the physical and technical support—the book—are ideally transparent in order to facilitate, and not disturb, phenomenological immersions.” (Mangen 2008, p. 406). In fact, even in a computer game it is generally desirable to have the interface become transparent (Clowes 2015a) or the sense of immersion is likely to be broken. In my discussion here I have therefore emphasized the different types of agency involved. There may be other circumstances where interface opacity is very useful as we shall go on to discuss.

  4. Mangen was already skeptical of the hypertext novel in her 2008 study as it appeared to foster a different sort of engagement i.e. not the desired hermeutic transparency.

  5. We know this precisely because of the surprise or notability of the few occasions when figures from antiquity attest to silent reading as something uncommon and notable. For example, Augustine was so surprised by Anselm’s ability to silently read that he wrote in his diary: “his eyes moved but not his tongue”. Julius Caesar is also supposed to have been a silent reader.

  6. Richard Heersmink points out (in private communication) that casting these very recent changes in how we read in the broader historical context risks committing the naturalistic fallacy. That is just because reading has transformed in the past does not mean that we should not be worried about contemporary changes in our reading behavior. This is a valid point, but the argument here is that reading has been different in the past, and that many contemporary critics seems not to notice. And in the meantime they idealize what reading is. This idealization gives us neither an accurate picture of what reading is, and also tends to obscure the ways that material reading substrates can support different sorts of readerly activities. The latter is of importance because we can only consciously re-engineer technologies to better meet our needs if we properly understand those needs.

  7. To reiterate the argument from Sect. 2, part of the claim about the decline in deep reading was that it might be triggering an overall cognitive shift towards, e.g. what Hayles (2007) calls shallow attention. Such claims are frequently made, but seldom as specific as that made by Hayles.

  8. All this said, for this user, there are several problems with the institutional framework in which the Kindle operates and how this tends to undermine certain aspects of how we traditionally swapped and lent books to one another, and indeed the very idea that we own a book. Problematic though this might be it is all fairly distantly related to the nature of our readerly minds, so we will pass over these questions here.

  9. NB – Malafouris, for one sees technologies as being material agents. There are complex questions to be worked through here that go beyond the scope of this paper.

  10. See Malafouris (2013), especially the section of Chap. 5 on “Making Numbers out of Clay”.

References

  • Ackerman R, Goldsmith M (2011) Metacognitive regulation of text learning: on screen versus on paper. J Exp Psychol Appl 17(1):18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson ML (2007) The massive redeployment hypothesis and the functional topography of the brain. Philos Psychol 20(2):143–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birkerts S (1994). The gutenberg elegies: the fate of reading in an electronic age. faber & faber. Boston

  • Birkerts S (2006). The gutenberg elegies: the fate of reading in an electronic age. Macmillan

  • Carr N (2008) Is Google making us stupid? Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education 107(2):89–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr N (2010) The shallows: how the internet is changing the way we think, read and remember. Atlantic Books, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Castro-Caldas A, Petersson KM, Reis A, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M (1998) The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain 121(6):1053–1063

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (1997) Being there: putting brain, body, and world together again. The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Clowes RW (2015a) The reality of the virtual self as interface to the social world. In: Fonseca J, Gonçalves J (eds) Philosophical perspectives on self. Peter Lang, Lisbon, pp 221–276

    Google Scholar 

  • Clowes RW (2015b) Thinking in the cloud: the cognitive incorporation of cloud-based technology. Philos Technol 28(2):261–296 Issue 2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clowes RW (In Press) Immaterial engagement: human agency within the cognitive ecology of the internet phenomenology and cognitive science (special issue on material engagement theory). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9560-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S (2009). Reading in the brain: The science and evolution of a human invention: Viking Pr

  • Dehaene S, Cohen L (2007) Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron 56(2):384–398

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene S, Cohen L, Sigman M, Vinckier F (2005) The neural code for written words: a proposal. Trends Cognitive Sci 9(7):335–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeStefano D, LeFevre JA (2007) Cognitive load in hypertext reading: a review. Comput Hum Behav 23(3):1616–1641

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dillon A (1992) Reading from paper versus screens: a critical review of the empirical literature. Ergonomics 35(10):1297–1326

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engelsing R (1969). Die Perioden der Lesergeschichte in der Neuzeit

  • Gerlach J, Buxmann P (2011). Investigating the acceptance of electronic books-the impact of haptic dissonance on innovation adoption. Paper presented at the ECIS

  • Greenfield S (2009, 23 April 2009). How Facebook Addiction is damaging your child’s brain: a leading neuroscientist’s chilling warning. The Daily Mail

  • Grzeschik K, Kruppa Y, Marti D, Donner P (2011) Reading in 2110-reading behavior and reading devices: a case study. Electron Library 29(3):288–302

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guillory J (2008) How scholars read. ADE Bulletin 146:8–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Haas C (2013). Writing technology: studies on the materiality of literacy: Routledge

  • Hayles NK (2007). Hyper and deep attention: The generational divide in cognitive modes. Profession, 187–199

  • Hayles NK (2012). How we think: Digital media and contemporary technogenesis: University of Chicago Press

  • Healy JM (2011). Endangered minds: why children don’t think and what we can do about it: Simon and Schuster

  • Hillesund T (2010). Digital reading spaces: How expert readers handle books, the Web and electronic paper. First Monday 15(4)

  • Hutchins E (2010) Cognitive ecology. Topics Cognitive Sci 2(4):705–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ihde D (1990) Technology and the lifeworld: from garden to earth: Indiana University Press

  • Ihde D (1991) Instrumental realism: the interface between philosophy of science and philosophy of technology (vol 626): Indiana University Press

  • Jacobson MJ, Spiro RJ (1995) Hypertext learning environments, cognitive flexibility, and the transfer of complex knowledge: an empirical investigation. J Educ Comput Res 12(4):301–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeong H (2012) A comparison of the influence of electronic books and paper books on reading comprehension, eye fatigue, and perception. Electron Library 30(3):390–408

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim H, Kim J (2013) Reading from an LCD monitor versus paper: teenagers’ reading performance. Int J Res Stud Educ Technol 2(1):1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Feldman MW (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behav Brain Sci 23:131–175

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lanier J (2010) You are not a gadget: a manifesto. Allen Lane, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu Z (2005) Reading behavior in the digital environment: changes in reading behavior over the past ten years. J Doc 61(6):700–712

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris L (2004). The cognitive basis of material engagement: where brain, body and culture conflate. Rethink Material Engagem Mind Mater World 53–61

  • Malafouris L (2013). How things shape the mind: MIT Press

  • Mangen A (2008) Hypertext fiction reading: haptics and immersion. J Res Read 31(4):404–419

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mangen A, Kuiken D (2014) Lost in an iPad: Narrative engagement on paper and tablet. Sci Study Lit 4(2):150–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Mangen A, van der Weel A (2015). Why don’t we read hypertext novels? Convergence: Int J Res New Media Technol 1354856515586042

  • Mangen A, Walgermo BR, Brønnick K (2013) Reading linear texts on paper versus computer screen: effects on reading comprehension. Int J Educ Res 58:61–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manguel A (1997) The history of reading. Penguin

  • Menary R (2014) Neural plasticity, neuronal recycling and niche construction. Mind Lang 29(3):286–303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miall DS, Dobson T (2001). Reading hypertext and the experience of literature. J Dig Inf 2(1)

  • Miranda T, Williams-Rossi D, Johnson KA, McKenzie N (2011). Reluctant readers in middle school: Successful engagement with text using the e-reader. Int J Appl Sci Technol 1(6)

  • Nicholas D, Rowlands I, Clark D, Huntington P, Jamali HR, Ollé C (2008) UK scholarly e-book usage: a landmark survey. Paper presented at the Aslib Proceedings

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pegado F, Comerlato E, Ventura F, Jobert A, Nakamura K, Buiatti M, Morais J (2014) Timing the impact of literacy on visual processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201417347

  • Petersson KM, Silva C, Castro-Caldas A, Ingvar M, Reis A (2007) Literacy: a cultural influence on functional left-right differences in the inferior parietal cortex. Eur J Neurosci 26(3):791–799

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross CS, Rothbauer PM (2006). Reading matters: What the research reveals about reading, libraries, and community: Libraries Unltd Incorporated

  • Smart PR, Heersmink R, Clowes RW (2017a) The Cognitive Ecology of the Internet. In: Cowley SJ, Vallée-Tourangeau F (eds) Cognition Beyond the Brain, 2nd Edition, Springer, pp 251–282

  • Smart PR, Clowes RW, Heersmink R (2017b) Minds online: the interface between web science, cognitive science and the philosophy of mind. Found Ttrends Web Sci 6(1–2):1–232. https://doi.org/10.1561/1800000026

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiro RJ, Jehng J-C (1990). Cognitive flexibility and hypertext: Theory and technology for the nonlinear and multidimensional traversal of complex subject matter. Cognition, education, and multimedia: exploring ideas in high technology, 205

  • Tenner E (2006). Searching for dummies. The New York Times, 12

  • Turkle S (2011) Alone together: why we expect more from technology and less from each other. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Tveit ÅK, Mangen A (2014) A joker in the class: teenage readers’ attitudes and preferences to reading on different devices. Library Inf Sci Res 36(3):179–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M (2007) Proust and the squid: the story and science of the reading brain. HarperCollins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M, Barzillai M (2009) The importance of deep reading. Educ Leadersh 66(6):32–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M, Ullman-Shade C, Gottwald S (2012). The emerging, evolving reading brain in a digital culture: implications for new readers, children with reading difficulties, and children without schools. J Cognitive Educ Psychol 11(3)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf M, Gottwald S, Galyean T, Morris R, Breazeal C (2014). The reading brain, Global Literacy

Download references

Acknowledgements

This production of this paper was funded by a grant from Fundação para a Ciencia e a Technologia (FCT) for the project: Virtualism and the Mind: Rethinking Presence, Representation and Self:—(SFRH/BPD/70440/2010). ​I want to thank Richard Heersmink for his detailed and pertinent comments, along with the comments of an anonymous reviewer which have much improved this paper. I also want to thank Paul Smart for some useful discussions about cognitive ecology. This paper is based upon an earlier conference paper presented at the Barcelona conference on social intelligence which is archived at http://ceur-ws.org/Vol-1283/paper_23.pdf. The work also benefitted from presentation in several drafts to the Lisbon Mind & Reasoning Group and also as part of a workshop on values for IFILNOVA and to the COGS group at the University of Sussex.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert W. Clowes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Clowes, R.W. Screen reading and the creation of new cognitive ecologies. AI & Soc 34, 705–720 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-017-0785-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00146-017-0785-5

Keywords

Navigation