Introduction

Non-native species invading areas beyond their native ranges are often a major threat to biodiversity (Lodge 1993; Mack et al. 2000; Bax et al. 2003) and their numbers are likely to increase due to worldwide traveling and transportation of goods (Lodge 1993; Hulme et al. 2008). Detrimental effects of invasive species on native species are well documented in the scientific literature, but we often lack a mechanistic understanding of the invasion success (Hayes and Barry 2008; Blackburn et al. 2011). A number of ecological and evolutionary hypotheses have been proposed aiming to explain the invasion success of non-native species (see Hufbauer and Torchin 2008), many of which focus on the role of biotic interactions. The ‘enemy release hypothesis’, for example, proposes that non-native species can benefit from missing predators (Keane and Crawley 2002; Colautti et al. 2004). Nevertheless, even if predators are present, the lack of shared evolutionary history between native prey and a non-native predator can facilitate the establishment of non-native predators because of lacking predator recognition, also known as ‘predator–prey naïveté hypothesis’ (Cox and Lima 2006; Carthey and Banks 2014).

Predation risk is one of the great driving forces of prey populations (Lima and Dill 1990) and invasive predators, in particular, are causing rapid extinctions or declines in many native prey species (Mooney and Cleland 2001; Carthey and Banks 2014). Besides predator–prey systems, predator-predator interactions can contribute to invasion success as well. Killing of other predators (i.e. intraguild predation) or interference with other predators (i.e. intraguild interference) is common in food webs and can hamper top-down control of predators (Polis et al. 1989; Arim and Marquet 2004). Studies on the invasion success of non-native predators should thus consider intraguild interactions (Vance-Chalcraft and Soluk 2005). So far, the empirical evaluation of hypotheses aimed to explain invasion success is biased towards plants, vertebrates and aquatic organisms while terrestrial insects received relatively less attention (Parker et al. 1999; Bax et al. 2003) despite the fact that they represent a large part of the alien fauna (Kenis et al. 2009).

In insects, chemical cues play an important role in intra- and interspecific communication (Howard and Blomquist 2005; Monnin 2006). Chemical recognition is best known in social insects, where cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs) serve as intraspecific recognition cues (Greene and Gordon 2003; Howard and Blomquist 2005), but also as cues to recognize mutualists (Lang and Menzel 2011; Menzel and Schmitt 2012) or to avoid competitors or predators (Geiselhardt et al. 2011; Mestre et al. 2014; Wüst and Menzel 2017). The composition of substances and compounds of CHC profiles varies between species (Geiselhardt et al. 2011; Menzel et al. 2017). If species use such specific chemical cues for species recognition, they either need to learn or adapt to the chemical cues of their interaction partners (Sih et al. 2010). However, if species recognition is based on general cues (i.e. chemical substances present in many different species) they should be able to recognize non-native predators despite missing co-evolutionary history (Sih et al. 2010). Consequently, the similarity of chemical cues between native and non-native species as well as the species’ ability to discriminate between different cue profiles is expected to influence interactions between native and non-native species and can thus contribute to the invasion success of non-native species (Sih et al. 2010).

Lady beetles have been intentionally redistributed across continents as pest control agents against aphids. In 1982, the multicolored Asian lady beetle, Harmonia axyridis, was introduced to Europe for the biological control of aphids in greenhouses and sold by biological control companies since 1995 (Brown et al. 2007). In 2002, first feral populations were sighted in the Netherlands, Belgium and Germany (Babendreier 2007). Since then, H. axyridis has spread over Central Europe and is now a dominant member of the lady beetle community in many European countries. H. axyridis displaces native lady beetles in Europe but also in North America (Elliott et al. 1996; Koch 2003; Brown et al. 2011; Roy et al. 2012). The rapid decline in the abundance of native lady beetle species in the last 30 years has become an increasing cause for concern (Alyokhin and Sewell 2004; Harmon et al. 2006).

So far, research on ecological effects of invasive lady beetles on native communities has concentrated on intraguild predation among lady beetles, on prey depletion, on body size and fecundity, on phenology as well as on habitat displacement (reviewed in Roy and Wajnberg 2008). More recently, the role of chemical protection (Kajita et al. 2010, 2014) and endoparasites (Vilcinskas et al. 2013) of invasive lady beetles for the invasion success has also been studied. In addition, differences in ant aggression towards native and non-native lady beetles could contribute to the invasion success of non-native lady beetles as well (Finlayson et al. 2009). Ants are known as predators, ecosystem engineers and for their important role in communities as hemipteran mutualists (Flatt and Weisser 2000; Stadler and Dixon 2005). Some ant species show aphid tending behavior: they feed on honeydew and defend aphids against various predators (Way 1963; Völkl et al. 1999; Stadler and Dixon 2005). Relying on aphids as a food source, lady beetles are competitors of ants and are thus particularly prone to ant aggression. Reduced intraguild interference with ants is expected to contribute to the invasion success of non-native lady beetles because of enhanced food access. So far, we lack the understanding to which extent ant aggression contributes to the invasion success of H. axyridis in Europe (Pell et al. 2008).

Here, we compared ant aggression towards lady beetles and lady beetle reaction upon contact with ants between native and non-native lady beetles in Europe. In our laboratory experiments, we confronted individuals of four native lady beetle species (Hippodamia variegata, Adalia bipunctata, Propylea quatuordecimpunctata, and Coccinella septempunctata) and the invasive lady beetle species H. axyridis with individuals of two co-occurring native ant species (Lasius niger and Myrmica rubra). In an additional experiment, we confronted ants with chemically manipulated lady beetle elytra. The elytra were either untreated, washed and thus expected to be free of chemical cues, or treated with chemical cues of the different lady beetle species. This dummy experiment allowed us to evaluate the role of chemical cues for ant aggression. Finally, we analyzed the chemical composition of CHCs using gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC–MS) to compare the similarity of chemical cues among the lady beetle species used in our behavioral assays. We expected (1) more ant aggression towards native lady beetles compared to invasive lady beetles; (2) more reactions of native lady beetles compared to invasive lady beetles if confronted with native ants. In our dummy experiment, we expected (3) fewer ant aggression towards washed elytra compared to untreated elytra and (4) comparable ant aggression between elytra with artificially applied lady beetle cues and untreated elytra. Regarding the chemical composition of lady beetle cues, we expected (5) a species-specific CHC profile for each lady beetle species included in our experiments.

Methods

Study species

Adult individuals of the five lady beetle species were collected from field margins in the agricultural landscape of Central Europe (Germany, France and Switzerland) from March till September 2017. Additional individuals of A. bipunctata were purchased (Bioinsecte, Adavalue SPRL, Othée, Belgium) as we could not find enough individuals in the wild. Lady beetle body size (from the end of the abdomen up to the front of the head) was measured under a binocular and lady beetles were weighted with a precision balance (H. axyridis: 5.24 ± 0.12 mm, 0.024 ± 0.001 g; H. variegata: 2.77 ± 0.07 mm, 0.009 ± 0.000 g; A. bipunctata: 3.32 ± 0.08 mm, 0.013 ± 0.000 g; P. quatuordecimpunctata: 3.17 ± 0.07 mm, 0.010 ± 0.000 g; C. septempunctata: 5.29 ± 0.13 mm, 0.041 ± 0.001 g). Small groups of lady beetles separated by species were kept in Petri dishes (ø 9 cm) with moistened cotton and stored in a climate cabinet (MLR-352H, Panasonic Corporation, Kadoma, Osaka, Japan; at 20 °C, 65% RH, 16/8 h day–night rhythm). The lady beetles were fed ad libitum with pea aphids Acyrthosiphon pisum. An initial population of aphids was provided by the Julius-Kühn-Institut (Braunschweig, Germany). Aphids were reared on bean plants Vicia faba (variety Sutton Dwarf; Kings Seeds, Essex, UK). Plants had to be renewed twice a week. Both aphids and plants were kept in climate cabinets (20 °C, 65% RH, 16/8 h day night rhythm).

Ant colonies of Lasius niger and Myrmica rubra were excavated in meadows and at forest edges in the vicinity of Marburg, Germany. Typical L. niger colonies consist on average of 14000 ant workers, M. rubra colonies typically consist of 1010 ant workers in average (Seifert 2007). All lady beetle species co-occurred at the sites where ants were collected, with H. axyridis being the most abundant lady beetle species during the study period and H. variegata and A. bipunctata being relatively rare at these sites. We always kept two ant colonies per species with brood during the whole study period. They were replaced by fresh colonies for the experiment with lady beetle elytra because only a few active workers and/or no brood were present anymore. In total, we ended up with 8 ant colonies for both experiments. Each colony was kept in a terrarium (20 cm × 20 cm × 30 cm) in the laboratory. The upper edge of the terraria were covered with Fluon® (Polytetrafluoroethylene dispersion) to prevent the ants from escaping. Water was provided in tubes clogged with cotton and the soil was moistened with a spray bottle twice a week. Ants were fed twice a week with honey and with dead house crickets Acheta domesticus.

Aggression experiments with living lady beetles

The behavioral assays were performed in Petri dishes (ø 9 cm) with Fluon®-covered walls to prevent the ants from escaping. Each Petri dish was only used once to prevent contamination with ant or lady beetle cues. For each trial, three ant workers were caught out of one colony and placed in the Petri dish. After an acclimatization time of 15 min, one adult lady beetle of the respective species was placed in the center of the Petri dish. During three minutes the following elements of ant behavior were quantified: avoidance of lady beetles, prolonged antennation, opening mandibles, chasing, grasping, biting, and stinging (see Finlayson et al. 2009). To compare the aggression strength of native ants against native and non-native lady beetles, we compared counts of strong aggressive behaviors that involved direct contact with lady beetles or that are associated with behavioral reactions in lady beetles (chasing, grasping, biting and stinging). Two alternative approaches, i.e. the aggression index proposed by Finlayson et al. (2009), and an analysis of the proportion of strong aggressive behaviors, are included in the supplementary material (S2) for comparison. In addition, the following elements of lady beetle behavior were quantified: continuing behavior, changing movement direction while walking, retracting legs or antennae, preening, turning on back, flailing legs, fluttering wings, backing, running away and flying away (see Finlayson et al. 2009). Here, behavioral elements leading to energetically costly increase in movement such as turning on back, flailing legs, fluttering wings, backing, running away and flying away were considered as strong reaction and are the basis of the analyses. As for ant aggression, we included two alternative analytical approaches of lady beetle reaction in the supplementary material (S2) for comparison. If behavioral elements continued for more than three seconds they were counted again to account for the duration (see e.g. Menzel et al. 2009). The frequencies of each behavioral element observed in our experiments are provided as supplementary material (S1) All behavioral assays were recorded with a video camera (LUMIX DMC-FZ300, Panasonic Corporation, Kadoma, Osaka, Japan) mounted on a tripod. The recordings were used for a slow-motion replay if many different behavioral elements occurred very quickly. For the aggression experiments with living lady beetle individuals, we compared five lady beetle species in combination with two ant species with at least 20 replicates per combination, resulting in 206 replicates. The order of lady beetles species identity during the experimental period was randomized.

To compare the frequency of ant aggression and lady beetle reaction between the ant and lady beetle species, we compared the counts of strong interactions (i.e. the sum of observed strong aggressive behaviors and the sum of strong reactions; see above). First, we calculated a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with ant and lady beetle species identity as well as the interaction between both fixed effects. The model assumed a Poisson error distribution and included ant colony identity as a random effect. In addition, we included an observation-level random effect to account for overdispersion (Harrison 2014). Since behavioral interactions differed between ant species, differences in interaction strength between the lady beetle species were analyzed for each ant species separately. In case of significant overall effects based on a subsequent χ2-test, a Tukey post-hoc test was applied to obtain pairwise contrasts between the lady beetle species with adjusted p-values (to account for multiple comparisons). All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.5.2 (R Development Core Team 2018).

Aggression experiments with manipulated elytra

We used lady beetle elytra as dummies to manipulate CHCs. Elytra can easily be removed from dead beetles. They contain no secretory glands (Pettersson 2012), and our analyses did not detect any other compounds than CHCs in elytra extracts. Ants can grasp and bite easily into these strongly sclerotized body parts. A number of 60 adult individuals per lady beetle species plus additional 200 H. axyridis individuals were killed in the freezer at approximately − 10 °C for 12 h. Thereafter, the dead lady beetles were defrosted and both elytra were broken off with a clean tweezer. The resulting 120 elytra per lady beetle species were divided into 80 elytra for the cue extraction and 40 elytra remained untreated. To obtain cue-free lady beetle elytra, 400 H. axyridis elytra were repeatedly immersed in clean 20 ml hexane or dichloromethane (i.e. to solve apolar and polar organic substances): hexane for ten min, dichloromethane for one hour, hexane for one hour, dichloromethane for ten min and again hexane for ten min. Between each step, the elytra were dried on clean paper towel for 30 s. Half of these cue-free elytra were later used as negative treatments (i.e. cues washed away) and the other half as positive treatment (i.e. adding lady beetle cues of the respective species). To obtain the cues of the different lady beetle species, 80 elytra of each lady beetle species were covered with 4.0 ml hexane for ten minutes. Afterwards, the solutions were transferred into smaller vials (4.0 ml, 15 mm × 45 mm) and were stored without cover to allow the hexane to evaporate. The CHCs were then resolved in 800 μl hexane.

In each aggression trial, three ants were confronted with one elytron of either of the following treatments: untreated elytron of the respective lady beetle species, cue-free elytron of H. axyridis with pure hexane (negative treatment), or cue-free elytron of H. axyridis with added cues of the respective lady beetle species solved in hexane (positive treatment). To this end, the elytron was held with a spring steel tweezer and 10.0 μl hexane was applied on the outer surface with a micropipette. After the hexane had dried, the elytron was turned and another 10.0 μl hexane was applied to the inner surface. For the positive treatment, the same procedure was applied but with the cue solution of the respective lady beetle species instead of pure hexane. In this dummy experiment, we ended up with 200 replicates with untreated elytra (20 replicates per lady beetle and ant species combination), 200 replicates of the negative treatment and 200 replicates of the positive treatment (with 20 replicates per cue-donator lady beetle and ant species combination) resulting in a total of 600 replicates. During the behavioral assays, ant aggression was quantified as described above. Counts of strong ant aggression were analyzed using a GLMM with Poisson error distribution. Treatment, ant species, and lady beetle species, as well as their interactions, entered the model as fixed effects with ant colony identity and observation-level as random effects (see analysis of the experiment with living lady beetles). Differences in ant aggression between the treatments were compared for each ant species separately. A Tukey post-hoc test was applied to obtain pairwise contrasts between the treatments.

Chemical analysis

CHC extracts for chemical analyses were obtained by immersing the two elytra of one freeze-killed lady beetle in 1.0 ml hexane for ten minutes. This was done for nine H. axyridis, eight H. variegata, four A. bipunctata, five P. quatuordecimpunctata, and seven C. septempunctata individuals. All extracts were concentrated under nitrogen flow and injected into a 7890A gas chromatograph coupled to a 5975C mass spectrometer (both Agilent Technologies Inc., Santa Clara, USA) in the split less mode at 250 °C. Helium was used as carrier gas (1.2 ml/min). The stationary phase was a capillary column (Phenomenex Zebron ZB5-HT Inferno, 30 m × 0.25 μm × 0.25 μm). Oven temperature was 60 °C for two min, then increased to 200 °C by 60 °C/min, and then increased to 320 °C by 4 °C/min, where it remained constant for ten minutes. We used an ionization current of 70 eV and scanned molecular fragments from 40 to 650 m/z. Data were acquired using the software MSD Chem Station E.02.02 (Agilent Technologies). We analyzed all hydrocarbons with a chain length > C20 and an average abundance of at least 0.5%; the abundance of all hydrocarbons < C20 totaled less than one percent of the total. Substances were identified based on retention time and diagnostic ions. Chemical differences between species were tested with a PERMANOVA (999 permutations, command adonis, R-package vegan, Oksanen et al. 2013) based on Bray–Curtis distances, which contained the relative abundances of all hydrocarbons as a dependent variable in a multivariate analysis.

Results

Ant aggression towards living lady beetles

The frequency of ant aggression towards lady beetles differed between lady beetle species (χ24,194 = 22.85, p < 0.001) and the frequency of L. niger aggression was higher than that of M. rubra (χ21,194 = 64.41, p < 0.001). We found no interactive effect of lady beetle and ant species identity on ant aggression frequency (χ24,194 = 3.94, p = 0.41). For L. niger, the frequency of aggression differed between lady beetle species (Fig. 1a; χ24,96 = 21.32, p < 0.001). Here, the frequency of ant aggression was higher towards C. septempunctata (z = 4.33, p < 0.001), P. quatuordecimpunctata (z = 3.69, p = 0.002), and A. bipunctata (z = 2.85, p = 0.04) compared to H. axyridis. For M. rubra, the frequency of ant aggression did not differ between lady beetle species (Fig. 1b; χ24,100 = 7.07, p = 0.13).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Frequency of strong aggression of a L. niger and b M. rubra towards the studied lady beetle species (Ha H. axyridis, Hv H. variegata, A2 A. bipunctata, P14 P. quatuordecimpunctata, C7 C. septempunctata) and frequency of strong reaction of the five lady beetle species upon encountering c L. niger and d M. rubra (mean ± SE). Different lowercase letters indicate statistical differences (α < 0.05). Letters in brackets indicate statistical trends (α < 0.1). Note that y-axes are on different scales

Reaction of lady beetles to ant encounters

The frequency of lady beetle reactions upon contact with ants differed between lady beetle species (χ24,194 = 31.54, p < 0.001) and the frequency of lady beetle reactions upon contact with L. niger was higher compared to that after confrontations with M. rubra (χ21,194 = 32.81, p < 0.001). We found no interactive effect of lady beetle and ant species identity on the frequency of lady beetle reaction (χ24,194 = 5.37, p = 0.25). Upon contact with L. niger, the frequency of lady beetle reactions differed between lady beetle species (Fig. 1c; χ24,96 = 21.67, p < 0.001). Here, C. septempunctata showed more reactions confronted with L. niger compared to H. axyridis (z = 4.09, p < 0.001), H. variegata (z = 3.46, p = 0.005) and A. bipunctata (z = 2.76, p = 0.05). In addition, the frequency of reactions tended to be higher in P. quatuordecimpunctata compared to H. axyridis (z = 2.59, p = 0.07). For encounters with M. rubra, the frequency of lady beetle reactions also differed between lady beetle species (Fig. 1d; χ24,100 = 15.34, p = 0.004). Here, lady beetle reaction was more frequent in C. septempunctata compared to H. axyridis (z = 3.10, p = 0.02) and A. bipunctata (z = 3.31, p = 0.01). We found no further pairwise differences in the frequency of lady beetle reactions upon contact with M. rubra among the remaining lady beetle species.

Aggression towards lady beetle CHCs

The frequency of ant aggression differed between the CHC treatments (Fig. 2; χ22,572 = 63.88, p < 0.001) but not between the two ant species (χ21,572 = 1.95, p = 0.16) or between the different lady beetle species (χ24,572 = 3.00, p = 0.56). We found a significant ant species/lady beetle species interaction (χ24,572 = 11.64, p = 0.02; see supplementary material S2 for species-wise analyses). The treatment/ant species interaction (χ22,572 = 3.70, p = 0.16), the treatment/lady beetle species interaction (χ28,572 = 13.34, p = 0.10), and the three-way interaction (χ28,572 = 4.96, p = 0.76) were not significant. For L. niger, the frequency of ant aggression differed between the CHC treatments (Fig. 2; χ22,289 = 50.68. p < 0.001) but not between lady beetle species (χ24,289 = 6.84, p = 0.14). We did not find a significant treatment/lady beetle species interaction (χ28,289 = 8.45, p = 0.39). Here, we found fewer ant aggression when chemical cues were removed compared to untreated elytra (z = 6.74, p < 0.001). The application of lady beetle cues of the respective species on cue-free H. axyridis elytra increased the frequency of ant aggression (z = 5.22, p < 0.001) and was similar to the aggression against untreated elytra (z = 1.65, p = 0.23). The frequency of aggression by M. rubra differed between CHC treatments (Fig. 2; χ22,285 = 20.29, p < 0.001) and tended to differ between lady beetle species (χ24,285 = 8.15, p = 0.09). We did not find a significant treatment/lady beetle species identity interaction (χ28,285 = 9.83, p = 0.28). Here, the frequency of ant aggression was reduced when chemical cues were removed compared to untreated elytra (z = 4.57, p < 0.001). The application of lady beetle cues of the respective species on cue-free elytra did not restore aggression compared to cue-free H. axyridis elytra (z = 1.83, p = 0.16). Consequently, the frequency of ant aggression towards elytra treated with lady beetle cues was lower compared to untreated elytra (z = 2.77, p = 0.02). Treatment comparisons separated by lady beetle species are provided as supplementary material (S2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Frequency of strong aggression (mean ± SE) of L. niger (black) and M. rubra (white) towards untreated elytra of the studied lady beetle species (Control), cue-free H. axyridis elytra (−Cues), and initially cue-free H. axyridis elytra bearing the cues of the respective lady beetle species (+Cues). Different letters indicate statistical differences (α < 0.05)

Chemical composition of cues

The composition of the chemical cues differed between lady beetle species (pseudo-F4,28 = 58.57, p < 0.001) revealing a distinct species-specific chemical profile for each lady beetle species (see Figs. 3 and 4; all pairwise-comparisons: pseudo-F ≥ 41.7, p ≤ 0.05). Here, cue composition was relatively similar between H. axyridis and H. variegata (multivariate distance between species centroids based on Bray–Curtis distances: 0.54). The profiles of both species were characterized by a very high proportion of alkenes, followed by alkadienes and n-alkanes. In contrast, the profile of P. quatuordecimpunctata was dominated by alkadienes (distance to the H. axyridis centroid: 0.73), while the profiles of C. septempunctata and A. bipunctata strongly differed from all previous species, having high proportions of monomethyl alkanes (as well as di- and trimethyl alkanes in the case of C. septempunctata) (distance to the H. axyridis centroid for both species: 0.86). Identified substances and chromatograms of each lady beetle species are provided as supplementary material (S1).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Schematic cuticular hydrocarbon (CHC) profile of the lady beetle species included in the behavioral assays. The bars represent the relative abundances of different substance classes at different chain lengths. Note that this representation does not show differences between hydrocarbons of the same hydrocarbon class and chain length

Fig. 4
figure 4

Ordination plot (Non-metric MultiDimensional Scaling with Bray–Curtis dissimilarity) based on the relative abundance of identified substances. Each symbol represents a lady beetle individual of the respective species (Ha H. axyridis, Hv H. variegata, A2 A. bipunctata, P14 P. quatuordecimpunctata, C7 C. septempunctata)

Discussion

Our behavioral experiments revealed that ant aggression as well as lady beetle reaction differed between ant and lady beetle species. Across lady beetle species, strong aggressive interactions were more often observed in L. niger than in M. rubra. In particular, the aggression of L. niger was more frequent against most native lady beetle species compared to the invasive H. axyridis. Additionally, C. septempunctata showed more reactions upon encountering both ant species compared to H. axyridis. Removing chemical cues from lady beetle elytra reduced aggression of both ant species and the addition of lady beetle cues on cue-free elytra increased the frequency of L. niger aggression but not that of M. rubra. Our chemical analyses showed a distinct species-specific CHC profile for each lady beetle species, which is a prerequisite for predator-predator naïveté.

Ant aggression and lady beetle reaction

The most evident pattern we observed was the higher frequency of strong L. niger aggression compared to that of M. rubra. L. niger is known to be more aggressive than M. rubra and often dominates M. rubra in direct encounters (Binz et al. 2014). In addition, ants of the genus Lasius are more strongly involved in trophobiosis with aphids and even overwinter aphids in their nests whereas ants of the genus Myrmica regularly prey on other arthropods including aphids (Seifert 2007). Consequently, L. niger is expected to be a stronger lady beetle competitor compared to M. rubra.

Less frequent L. niger aggression (fewer grasping and biting) against H. axyridis compared to most native lady beetle species tested—except for H. variegata—is in line with our expectation. These results suggest that L. niger lack the ability to recognize non-native H. axyridis and to react accordingly. Furthermore, H. axyridis is an equally voracious aphid predator compared to C. septempunctata (Ünlü et al. 2020; Bertleff et al. 2021) and consequently an equally strong competitor. Thus, one would expect a selection pressure towards ants to become equally aggressive to H. axyridis in the long term. If recognition is learned, H. axyridis should elicit strong aggression, since it was by far the most common lady beetle at the sites of ant collection and some ant workers used in our study might have encountered it before. However, the ants were much less aggressive towards this species. So far, it is largely unknown whether ant responses to different insect species are innate or learnt. Several studies reported that ants can be conditioned to certain insect odors (Bos et al. 2012; Dupuy et al. 2006), suggesting that at least some responses can be learned. However, it is equally possible that there are default innate responses to certain cues, a scenario which would pose a benefit for invasive species if they differ in cues. Further experiments with completely lady beetle-naïve ants would be required to test whether the recognition ability is learned or innate.

Regarding the role of intraguild interactions in the ant-lady beetle-aphid system, the vast majority of studies focuses on intraguild predation between lady beetles (reviewed by Pell et al. 2008), whereas information about differences in ant aggression against lady beetles is scarce. To our knowledge, the only multispecies comparison including native and non-native lady beetle species has been conducted by Finlayson et al. (2009). In their study aggression of M. rubra workers tending potato aphids Macrosiphum euphorbiae was compared between North American, European, and the Asian lady beetle species H. axyridis. Similar to our results, Finlayson et al. (2009) found no differences in the intensity of M. rubra aggression against novel H. axyridis and co-evolved C. septempunctata. Lacking lady beetle species discrimination in M. rubra but not in L. niger might again be explained by their involvement in trophobiosis with aphids (i.e. more competitive encounters with lady beetles), which is more pronounced in the genus Lasius than in the genus Myrmica (Seifert 2007).

The reaction of the different lady beetle species towards ants mirrored to some extent differences in the frequency of ant aggression among the lady beetle species, i.e. more aggressive interactions led to more escape behavior. In particular, C. septempunctata showed twice as many strong reactions if confronted with both ant species compared to H. axyridis. Although L. niger aggressions were more frequent against C. septempunctata, more frequent reactions suggest that C. septempunctata is able to effectively avoid ant aggression. Earlier studies under more natural conditions indicate that C. septempunctata is quite successful in avoiding ants (Sloggett et al. 1998) and is better protected with alkaloids compared to A. bipunctata (Marples 1993). Not only H. axyridis but also A. bipunctata showed fewer reactions upon contact with M. rubra compared to C. septempunctata. This pattern is in contrast with our hypothesis and suggest species-specific responses among lady beetle species (e.g. depending on body size-related competition for aphids or other exclusion mechanisms exist under more natural conditions). In summary, our laboratory experiments revealed more frequent aggression of L. niger compared to M. rubra and provide evidence that L. niger aggression as well as lady beetle reaction less frequent in the invasive H. axyridis compared to the equally voracious native lady beetle C. septempunctata—with potential implications for their invasion success in Europe.

Implications of ant aggression for the invasion success of H. axyridis

Ants have pervasive and complex effects on terrestrial ecosystems by influencing community structure but also by hampering top-down control via consumptive and non-consumptive effects (Stadler and Dixon 2005; Sanders et al. 2011; Mestre et al. 2014; Penn and Athey 2020). For example, the presence of North American ants reduced soybean aphid predation by Orius insidiosus (Hemiptera: Anthocoridae) and H. axyridis and led to a tenfold increase in aphid numbers (Herbert and Horn 2008). As a result of ant aggression, lady beetles only feed on ant-tended aphids when untended prey is scarce (Sloggett and Majerus 2000). In the presence of L. niger, reduced ant aggression—possibly due to predator-predator naiveté—may reinforce the competitive advantage of the invasive H. axyridis over C. septempunctata. However, experiments under more natural conditions would be necessary to infer competitive advantages of invasive lady beetles due to release form intraguild interference by ants. Differences in ant aggression between different lady beetle species could change if ants are actually tending aphids (Way 1963) and lady beetle species could differ in their foraging preference. For example, the myrmecophilous Coccinella magnifica received similar ant aggression compared to C. septempunctata on trails but only C. magnifica fed upon tended aphids (Sloggett et al. 1998). Given the strong context dependency in ant-lady beetle interactions, we see the need for more natural experiments including ants, lady beetles, and aphids to compare differences in aphid predation rates among lady beetle species in the presence and absence of ants. Such experiments would ideally include different lifestages of lady beetles as well as different competing lady beetle species. Nonetheless, the experimental approach presented here is very well suited to study behavioral responses to native and non-native lady beetle species since they allow large replicate numbers across multiple species and enable detailed behavioral analyses (e.g. to disentangle chemical and behavioral cues underlying aggression in ants, see Foitzik et al. 2007; Steiner et al. 2007; Menzel et al. 2009).

The role of chemical cues for intraguild interference with ants

Ants were aggressive against lady beetle elytra, albeit less so than against living beetles. Removal of chemical cues led to a strong reduction of ant aggression. This indicates that chemical cues are highly relevant for intraguild interactions between lady beetles and ants. This is not surprising, given that for ants, cuticular chemical cues, such as cuticular hydrocarbons, are the basis for the recognition of nestmates (Howard and Blomquist 2005) and mutualistic interaction partners (Lang and Menzel 2011; Menzel and Schmitt 2012). The addition of lady beetle cues on initially cue-free elytra led to similar L. niger aggression compared to the untreated elytra demonstrating that chemical cues elicit aggression of L. niger. Surprisingly, the addition of lady beetle cues did not increase the frequency of aggression by M. rubra. The aggression of M. rubra was generally less frequent than that of L. niger, which might be due to differences in their food preference (see discussion above). The chemical treatment might further weaken behavioral responses because the cues are not presented in the entirely natural form, and hence trigger weaker responses. In addition, the lady beetle extract might have contained defense secretions, which might have remained undetected in our chemical analyses (e.g. coccinelline). Such compounds might have deterred ants to some extent and thus weakened ant aggression. However, it seems unlikely that it affected M. rubra but not L. niger. At least for L. niger, aggression could be restored by re-application of CHC extracts reconfirming their role for species recognition. However, in contrast to the live beetles, ant aggression did not differ between the untreated elytra of the respective lady beetle species (see Supplementary Material S2, Fig. S2e). Hence, the CHCs alone might not fully explain differences in ant aggression, suggesting that behavioral responses by the lady beetles further modulate ant aggression. Finlayson et al. (2009) observed that ant aggression was higher if some lady beetle species had more exposed body parts on which ants could grasp. Albeit CHCs play an important role in species recognition, analyses of lady beetle CHCs targeted at sex-specific differences revealed that male and female lady beetles are coated by the same blend of CHCs (Hemptinne et al. 1998). Hemptinne et al. (1998) suggest that lady beetles rely on behavior for discrimination between males and females. Moreover, generally weak ant aggression against elytra might mitigate differences in aggression between lady beetle species that occur under natural conditions (i.e. against living lady beetles).

Our chemical analyses of the lady beetle cues revealed a unique CHC profile for each lady beetle species. All species were significantly different from each other, with only few shared substances among all lady beetle species. The shared substances (e.g. n-C22, n-C23, n-C25, and n-C27) are commonly found in many insect species and, due to their high melting point, have been related to waterproofing rather than to recognition (Menzel et al. 2019). Notably, the species showed a remarkable chemical diversity even concerning the dominant substance classes: while C. septempunctata and A. bipunctata profiles were dominated by monomethyl alkanes, H. axyridis and H. variegata mostly possessed alkenes, and P. quatuordecimpunctata mostly contained alkadienes. All of these compounds have vastly different physical properties (Menzel et al. 2019). The adaptive value of these differences still remains to be found. Interestingly, the main compound in our H. axyridis samples, a C25-alkene, was not reported in conspecific samples from Japan (Magro et al. 2010a), which might be due to between-population differences in this species. Usually, CHC profiles are qualitatively quite invariant within a species (Kather and Martin 2012, Sprenger and Menzel 2020), which is why they are highly useful for chemotaxonomy, i.e. to distinguish closely related or cryptic species. So far, lady beetle CHCs have mainly been studies in the context of mating (Hemptinne et al. 1998; Legrand et al. 2019), as oviposition deterring pheromone in the tracks of the larvae (Hemptinne et al. 2001), but also the role of CHCs on lady beetle eggs for cannibalism and intraguild predation has been studied (Hemptinne et al. 2000). The species-specific chemical profiles of lady beetles also mean that there is likely no general chemical lady beetle cue (i.e. substances present on all aphidophagous lady beetles). Rather, ants have to recognize each species individually, be it by learning or by evolutionary adaptation. Cue dissimilarity between native and non-native predators as well as species-specific recognition cues are a prerequisite for predator-predator naïveté against invasive predators (Sih et al. 2010). Interestingly, we found no differences in the frequency of ant aggression nor in lady beetle reaction between H. axyridis and H. variegata. In our study, this species pair is also the most similar one concerning their CHC classes, which might be due to their close phylogenetic relatedness; Hippodamia and Harmonia seem to be sister genera (Magro et al. 2010b; Escalona et al. 2017). Thus, native ants might recognize the invasive H. axyridis as a Hippodamia-like lady beetle and behave accordingly. However, a larger species set would be required to formally link chemical cue similarity or phylogenetic relatedness with aggression strength.

Conclusions

Our behavioral experiments revealed differences in ant aggression and lady beetle reaction between ant and lady beetle species. In particular, L. niger showed more aggression towards lady beetles compared to M. rubra, and aggression of L. niger was more frequent towards most native lady beetle species compared to the invasive H. axyridis. The frequency of this intraguild interference is influenced by the presence of chemical cues on the surface of lady beetles. CHC profiles of the lady beetles were species-specific and highly divergent, and we could not find any lady beetle-specific pattern or compound that would not occur in other insects as well (see e.g. Kather and Martin 2015; Sprenger and Menzel 2020). Moreover, secondary metabolites (e.g. defensive compounds; Haulotte et al. 2012) differ between lady beetle species. Therefore, the existence of general chemical lady beetle cues seems unlikely making it necessary for ants to recognize lady beetle species individually, although it is possible that they might lump chemically similar species.

Our aggression assays with L. niger thus provide support for the theoretical framework proposed by Sih et al. (2010) suggesting predator-naïveté against novel predators if interaction partners rely on species-specific cues. However, some native lady beetle species responded similarly to both ant species compared with the invasive H. axyridis, and also received relatively little ant aggression. This contradicts a simple native/non-native dichotomy. Note however, that H. axyridis is about as large as C. septempunctata, and both species have similarly high aphid consumption rates, compared to the other three species, which are smaller and less voracious (Ünlü et al. 2020; Bertleff et al. 2021). Therefore, when accounting for further variables like aphid consumption rate, the low aggression towards the invasive H. axyridis is conspicuous. Less frequent L. niger aggression and lady beetle reaction likely benefits the non-native lady beetle H. axyridis when competing with the equally voracious native C. septempunctata for ant-tended aphids and might ultimately contribute to its invasion success.