Skip to main content

Neural Correlates of Gender, Culture, and Race and Implications to Embodied Thinking in Mathematics

  • Chapter
Book cover Towards Equity in Mathematics Education

Part of the book series: Advances in Mathematics Education ((AME))

  • 2513 Accesses

Abstract

In this chapter, I discuss neuroscience research and selected findings that are relevant to mathematics education. What does it mean, for example, to engage in a neuroscientific analysis of symbol reference? I also discuss various research programs in neuroscience that have useful implications in mathematics education research. Further, I provide samples of studies conducted within and outside mathematics education that provide a neural grounding of gender, culture, and race. The chapter closes with three brief implications of neuroscientific work in mathematics education research, in general, and in individual- and intentional-embodied cognition in mathematical thinking and learning, in particular.

Writing of this chapter has been supported by a grant from the National Science Foundation (DRL Grant #0448649) awarded to the author. The ideas explored and expressed in this chapter are those of the author and do not reflect the views of the foundation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Certainly motives behind interests in neuroscience outside education depend on stakeholder contexts. Hacking (2004) articulated medical interests as an example. Neuroscientific findings and programs in the in nonmedical issues that bear on national security (National Research Council 2008) are also of interest to federal and military agencies in the USA. In 2008, the NRC published the document, Emerging Cognitive Neurosciences and Related Technologies, in which an attempt is made to address ways in which neuroscientific knowledge could be used to eventually develop usable “future warfighting applications” (p. 14) for the intelligence community. Such applications would have neuroscience associated with the following tasks: (1) “read” the “cognitive states and intentions of persons of interest;” (2) “enhance” the “cognitive capacities” of soldiers (how to make them learn faster and process information more quickly and precisely than usual, how to help them make correct decisions when engaged in battle); (3) “control” the “states and intentions” of oneself (e.g., pain, fear) and others (e.g., “disrupt” an “enemy’s motivation to fight”), and: (4) “drive devices” via “cognitive states” (e.g., using white noise to impair senses, using neuropsychopharmacology to develop drugs that “target specific sensory receptors”) (pp. 16–17).

  2. 2.

    Sensory memory lasts for a few seconds and quickly keeps and discards copies of immediately acquired visual and auditory information. Short-term memory (STM) is a short-term storage of information transferred from sensory memory and does not manipulate the acquired information. STM provides a space for engaging in quick calculations and holds visual and auditory information. Working memory (WM) is the active operational component in STM. It actively processes information acquired in STM and is central in the development of language, reading, mathematics, and problem solving. WM also deals with attentional resources in STM such as the ability to concentrate on one aspect of a target object and shutting off others. Long-term memory (LTM) stores information over periods of time and is organized via schemes that join together to form new knowledge structures. Readers are referred to Menon (2010) for an extended discussion of the neuroanatomical correlates of working memory and other relevant cognitive processes relevant in the development of mathematical thinking and skills.

  3. 3.

    In particular, it is worth noting the interesting methodological reflections of Poldrack (2006) and Henson (2006) concerning ways functional neuroimaging data are employed in developing arguments that they term as forward and reverse inferences, which involve establishing relationships between cognitive functioning and brain activation; for ethical issues involving neural-based reverse inferences, see Poldrack 2008. Forward inferences are deductively valid, and proceeds from assessing neural activity on the basis of performing certain cognitive tasks. Reverse inferences are deductively invalid since they involve making conclusions about cognitive functioning on the basis of brain activation. For Poldrack (2006),

    cognitive neuroscience is generally interested in a mechanistic understanding of the neural processes that support cognition rather than the formulation of deductive laws. To this end, reverse inference might be useful in the discovery of interesting new facts about the underlying mechanisms. Indeed, philosophers have argued that this kind of reasoning (termed ‘abductive inference’ by Peirce), is an essential tool for scientific discovery” (p. 60).

    However, Henson’s (2006) point below is a reminder about being mindful of neuroscientific claims:

    [I]t is important to think carefully about the type of inferences that can be made from functional neuroimaging data… only by making these caveats and assumptions explicit, and criticizing them, will we be able to assess the real value of functional neuroimaging for cognitive science” (p. 68).

  4. 4.

    Kaufmann (2008) points out that current fMRI experiments are restricted to 5-year-old children and older because “fMRI technique requires participants to be awake and respond to stimuli presented in the (narrow and very noisy) scanner environment while simultaneously task-processing related changes in the blood oxygen consumption in different brain regions are recorded” (pp. 2–3).

  5. 5.

    See Varga et al. (2010) and Nieder (2005) for syntheses of research comparing human and animal competence involving concepts of counting, cardinality (numerical quantity), and order (rank) from neuroscientific and neurobiological perspectives. For fMRI comparisons between children and adults involving different aspects of arithmetical processing, see Rocha et al. (2005) and Kawashima et al. (2004). See Ansari (2009) for a review analysis of results drawn from various neuroscientific studies that focused on developmental disorders and difficulties involving numerical cognition and relevant mathematical processes.

  6. 6.

    Current interests in the implications of approximate number sense are linked to its possible reverse-inferential relationship to school mathematics achievement for both children and adults. For example, based on a longitudinal assessment of 64 14-year-old children with normal development that started in kindergarten, Halberda et al. (2008) established a strong correlation between individual children’s approximate number sense and their past scores on standardized school mathematics achievement tests. Mazzocco et al. (in press) established a strong correlation between domain-specific deficits in approximate number processing and persistently deficient mathematics achievement among children with mathematical learning disabilities (i.e. those who scored below the 10th percentile in a mathematics achievement test).

  7. 7.

    See Gentilucci and Corballis (2006) for an interesting and though-provoking neuroscientific-based account of the evolution of speech and language from manual gestures to vocal communications (i.e. “gestural-origins theory;” an account that differs from the typical sound-to-language perspective).

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2010). Conceptual challenges and directions for social neuroscience. Neuron, 65(6), 752–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D. & Fiske, S. (2010). Cultural neuroscience. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 72–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J., Qin, Y., Sohn, M., Stenger, V., & Carter, C. (2003). An information-processing model of the BOLD response in symbol manipulation tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 241–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. & Reid, C. (2009). Don’t forget about levels of explanation. Cortex, 45, 560–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D. (2005). Paving the way towards meaningful interactions between neuroscience and education. Developmental Science, 8(6), 466–467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D. (2008). Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 278–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D. (2009). Neurocognitive approaches to developmental disorders of numerical and mathematical cognition: The perils of neglecting the role of development. Learning and Individual Differences, 20, 123–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ansari, D., Coch, D., & De Smedt, B. (2011). Connecting education and cognitive neuroscience: Where will the journey take us? Educational Philosophy and Theory, 43(1), 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandettini, P. (2009). Seven topics in functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, 8(3), 371–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechtel, W. (2002). Aligning multiple research techniques in cognitive neuroscience: Why is it important? Philosophy of Science, 69, 48–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. (2005). The independence of language and mathematical reasoning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(9), 3177–3178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruer, J. (1999). In search of… brain-based education. Phi Delta Kappan, 80(9), 649–657.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., Reeve, R., Reynolds, F., & Lloyd, D. (2008). Numerical thought with and without words: Evidence from indigenous Australian children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(35), 13179–13184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. (2006). Defining mathematics educational neuroscience. In Proceedings of the 28th annual meeting of the North American chapter of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 2, 442–449). Merida, Mexico: Universidad Pedagogica Nacional.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantlon, J., Brannon, E., Carter, E., & Pelphrey, K. (2006). Functional imaging of numerical processing in adults and 4-year old children. Public Library of Science Biology, 4(5), 844–854.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantlon, J., Platt, M., & Brannon, E. (2009). Beyond the number domain. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2), 83–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cela-Conde, C., Ayala, F., Munar, E., Maestu, F., Nadal, M., Capo, M., del Rio, D., Lopez-Ibor, J., Ortiz, T., Mirasso, C., & Mary, G. (2009). Sex-related similarities and differences in the neural correlates of beauty. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(10), 3847–3852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cerulo, K. (2010). Mining the intersections of cognitive sociology and neuroscience. Poetics, 38, 115–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J. (2011). Where does human diversity come from? In M. Brockman (Ed.), Future science: Essays from the cutting edge (pp. 236–248). New York: Vintage Books, Random House, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J., Li, Z., & Harada, T. (2008). Cultural neuroscience of consciousness: From visual perception to self-awareness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(10), 58–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christodoulou, J., & Gaab, N. (2009). Using and misusing neuroscience in education-related research. Cortex, 45, 555–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D., & Sarama, J. (2007). Early childhood mathematics learning. In F. Lester, Jr. (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 461–556). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coch, D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Thinking about mechanisms is crucial to connecting neuroscience and education. Cortex, 45, 546–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Hevia, M. D., Vallar, G., & Girelli, L. (2008). Visualizing numbers in the mind’s eye: The role of visuo-spatial processes in numerical abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1361–1372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 218–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delazer, M., Domahs, F., Bartha, L., Brenneis, C., Lochy, A., Trieb, T., & Benke, T. (2003). Learning complex arithmetic—An fMRI study. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 76–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Eickhoff, S., Kellermann, T., Falkenberg, D., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2010). Multidimensional assessment of emphatic abilities: Neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 67–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eberhardt, J. (2005). Imaging race. American Psychologist, 60(2), 181–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Editorial (2003). Scanning the social brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6(12), 1239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Editorial (2005). Separating science from stereotype. Nature Neuroscience, 8(3), 253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eliot, L. (2010). The truth about boys and girls. Scientific American Mind, 21(2), 22–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, T., Code, C., & Herrmann, M. (2007). Common brain regions underlying different arithmetic operations as revealed by conjunct fMRI-BOLD activation. Brain Research, 1172, 93–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. (2007). On prejudice & the brain. Daedalus, 136(1), 156–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuson, K. (2009). Avoiding misinterpretations of Piaget and Vygotsky: Mathematical teaching without learning, learning without teaching, or helpful learning-path teaching? Cognitive Development, 24, 343–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geake, J. (2005). Educational neuroscience and neuroscientific education: In search of a mutual middle-way. Research Intelligence, 92(3), 10–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (2000). Available light: Anthropological reflections on philosophical topics. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M. & Corballis, M. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 949–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, S., & Burgess, P. (2008). Social and nonsocial functions of rostral prefrontal cortex: Implications for education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(3), 148–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobbini, M., & Haxby, J. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia, 45, 32–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., & Dolan, R. (2004). Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition, 93, B109–B121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Buchel, C., Frith, C., & Dolan, R. (2000). Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllogistic reasoning. NeuroImage, 12, 504–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1997). The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. NeuroReport, 8, 1305–1310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goel, V., Gold, B., Kapur, S., & Houle, S. (1998). Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 293–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2004). Neuroscience and education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2005). The brain in the classroom? The state of the art. Developmental Science, 8(6), 467–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2008). Principles of learning, implications for teaching: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 42(3–4), 381–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2009). Mind, brain, and literacy: Biomarkers as usable knowledge for education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 3(3), 176–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R., Ansari, D., Schneider, M., De Smedt, B., Hannula, M., & Stern, E. (Eds.) (2010). Cognitive neuroscience and mathematics learning. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(6), 511–663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grön, G., Wunderlich, A., Spitzer, M., Tomczak, R., & Riepe, M. (2000). Brain activation during human navigation: Gender-different neural networks as substrate of performance. Nature Neuroscience, 3(4), 404–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I. (2004). The race against time. New Statesman, 133(4698), 26–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halberda, J., Mazzocco, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in nonverbal number acuity correlated with maths achievement. Nature, 455, 665–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, V., & Stewart, C. (2002). What do brain data really show? Philosophy of Science, 69, 72–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H., & Gabrieli, J. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 12–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henson, R. (2006). Forward inference using functional neuroimaging: Dissociations versus associations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 64–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, C., Miyamoto, K., & Della-Chiesa, B. (2008). Brain research, learning, and emotions: Implications for education research, policy, and practice. European Journal of Education, 43(1), 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard-Jones, P. (2008). Philosophical challenges for researchers at the interface between neuroscience and education. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 42(3–4), 361–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, E., Diester, I., Cantlon, J., Ansari, D., van Opstal, F., & Troiani, V. (2008). The evolution of numerical cognition: From number neurons to linguistic quantifiers. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(46), 11819–11824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J., & Mertz, J. (2009). Gender, culture, and mathematics performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(22), 8801–8807.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T., & Bartholow, B. (2009). The neural correlates of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(12), 524–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, S., & Nieder, A. (2009). Notation-independent representation of fractions in the human parietal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(14), 4652–4657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kadosh, R., Lammertyn, J., & Izard, V. (2008). Are numbers special? An overview of chronometric, neuroimaging, developmental, and comparative studies of magnitude representation. Progress in Nuerobiology, 84, 132–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, S., Walther, S., Nennig, E., Kronmuller, K., Mundt, C., Weisbrod, M., Stippich, C., & Vogeley, K. (2008). Gender-specific strategy use and neural correlates in a spatial perspective taking task. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2524–2531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufmann, L. (2008). Neural correlates of number processing and calculation: Developmental trajectories and educational implications. In A. Dowker (Ed.), Mathematical difficulties: Psychology and intervention (pp. 1–12). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kawashima, R., Taira, M., Okita, K., Inoue, K., Tajima, N., Yoshida, H., Sasaki, T., Sugiura, M., Watanabe, J., & Fukuda, H. (2004). A functional fMRI study of simple arithmetic—A comparison between children and adults. Cognitive Brain Research, 18, 225–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, K., & Menon, V. (2009). Gender differences in the functional and structural neuroanatomy of mathematical cognition. NeuroImage, 47, 342–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, C., Glover, G., & Temple, E. (2007). Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of “Theory of Mind” in American and Japanese children. Brain Research, 1164, 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., Lim, Z. Y., Yeong, S., Ng, S. F., Venkatraman, V., & Chee, M. (2007). Strategic differences in algebraic problem solving: Neuroanatomical correlates. Brain Research, 1155, 163–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzocco, M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (in press). Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability. Child Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menon, V. (2010). Developmental cognitive neuroscience of arithmetic: Implications for learning and education. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(6), 515–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monti, M., Parsons, L., & Osherson, D. (2011). Thought beyond language: Neural dissociation of algebra and natural language. Retrieved August 8, 2011, online at http://www.princeton.edu/~osherson/papers/Monti9.pdf.

  • National Research Council (2008). Emerging cognitive neuroscience and related technologies: Report of the committee on military and intelligence methodology for emergent neurophysiological and cognitive/neural science research in the next two decades. Washington: National Research Council.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nieder, A. (2005). Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 177–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieder, A. (2009). Prefrontal cortex and the evolution of symbolic reference. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obersteiner, A., Dresler, T., Reiss, K., Vogel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Fallgatter, A. (2010). Bringing brain imaging to the school to assess arithmetical problem solving: Chances and limitations in combining educational and neuroscientific research. ZDM—The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(6), 541–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development [OECD] (2007). Understanding the brain: The birth of a learning science. Paris, France: OECD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B., Snyder, K., & Roberts, Jr., R., (2007). Developmental cognitive neuroscience: Origins, issues, and prospects. Developmental Review, 27, 428–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S., & Fiez, J. (1993). The processing of single words studied with positron emission tomography. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 16, 661–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E., & Thomas, L. (2003). Race, behavior, and the brain: The role of neuroimaging in understanding complex social behaviors. Political Psychology, 24(4), 747–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 59–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. (2008). The role of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience: Where do we stand? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18, 223–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Edwards, L., & Arzarello, F. (Eds.) (2009). Special issue on gestures and multimodality in the construction of mathematical meaning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 91–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rocha, F., Rocha, A., Massad, E., & Menezes, R. (2005). Brain mappings of the arithmetic processing in children and adults. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 359–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg-Lee, M., Lovett, M., & Anderson, J. (2009). Neural correlates of arithmetic calculation strategies. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neurosciences, 9(3), 270–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanfey, A., Rilling, J., Aronson, J., Nystrom, L., & Cohen, J. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–1758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlöglmann, W. (2003). Can neuroscience help us better understand affective reactions in mathematics learning? In M. A. Mariotti (Ed.), CERME 3: Third conference of the European society for research in mathematics education (pp. 1–10). Bellaria, Italy: CERME.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, M., Goode, A., Koedinger, K., Stenger, V., Fissell, K., & Carter, C. (2004). Behavioral equivalence, but not neural equivalence— Neural evidence of alternative strategies in mathematical thinking. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 1193–1194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, Z., della Chiesa, B., Hinton, C., & Fischer, K. (2011). Ethical issues in educational neuroscience: Raising children in a brave new world. In J. Illes & B. Sahakian (Eds.), Oxford handbook of neuroethics (pp. 803–822). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szücs, D., & Goswami, U. (2007). Educational neuroscience: Defining a new discipline for the study of mental representations. Mind, Brain, and Education, 1(3), 114–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y., Zhang, W., Chen, K., Feng, S., Ji, Y., Shen, J., Reiman, E. & Liu, Y. (2006). Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(28), 10755–10780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terao, A., Koedinger, K., Sohn, M., Qin, Y., Anderson, J., & Carter, C. (2004). An fMRI study of the interplay of symbolic and visuo-spatial systems in mathematical reasoning. In Proceedings of the twenty-sixth annual conference of the cognitive science society. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thagard, P. (2010). How brains make mental models. In L. Magnani, W. Carnielli, & C. Pizzi (Eds.), Model-based reasoning in science and technology: Abduction, logic, and computational discovery (pp. 447–461). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, A., Harris, L., & Fiske, S. (2006). Toward socially inspired social neuroscience. Brain Research, 1079, 76–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tommerdahl, J. (2010). A model for bridging the gap between neuroscience and education. Oxford Review of Education, 36(1), 97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Bavel, J., Packer, J., & Cunningham, W. (2008). The neural substrates of in-group bias. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1131–1139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varga, M., Pavlova, O., & Nosova, S. (2010). The counting function and its representation in the parietal cortex in humans and animals. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 40(2), 185–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varley, R., Klessinger, N., Romanowski, C., & Siegal, M. (2005). Agrammatic but numerate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(9), 3519–3524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. (2009). Three problems in the marriage of neuroscience and education. Cortex, 45, 544–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zamarian, L., Ischebeck, A., & Delazer, M. (2009). Neuroscience of learning arithmetic—Evidence from brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 909–925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zan, R., Brown, L., Evans, J., & Hannula, M. (Eds.) (2006). Special issue on affect in mathematics education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 63(2), 113–234.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ferdinand Rivera .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rivera, F. (2012). Neural Correlates of Gender, Culture, and Race and Implications to Embodied Thinking in Mathematics. In: Forgasz, H., Rivera, F. (eds) Towards Equity in Mathematics Education. Advances in Mathematics Education. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-27702-3_47

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics